WWW.DISSERTATION.XLIBX.INFO
FREE ELECTRONIC LIBRARY - Dissertations, online materials
 
<< HOME
CONTACTS



Pages:   || 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |

«Discrimination Nets as Psychological Models* LAWRENCE BARSALOU W. Emory University GORDONH. BOWER Stanford University Simulations of human cognitive ...»

-- [ Page 1 ] --

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 8, 1-26 (1984)

Discrimination Nets

as Psychological Models*

LAWRENCE BARSALOU

W.

Emory University

GORDONH. BOWER

Stanford University

Simulations of human cognitive processes often employ discrimination nets to

model the access of permanent memory. We consider two types of dixrimina-

.

~

tion nets-EPAM and positive-proper&only nets-and argue that they have insufficient psychologi&l validity. ~hei;deficiencies arise from negative prop- erties, insufficient sensitivity to the discriminativeness of properties, extreme sensitivity to missing or incorrect properties, inefficiency in representingmul- tiple knowledgedomains, and seriality. We argue that these deficiencies stem from a high degree of test contingency in utilizing property information during acquisition and memory search. Discrimination nets are compared to other models that have less or no test contingency (e.g., PANDEMONIUM) and that thereby avoid the problems of discrimination nets. We propose that under- standing test contingency and discovering psychologically valid ways to im- plement i t will be central to understanding and simulating memory indexing in human cognition.

In reviewing information processing models in the Annual Review of Psy- chology, Simon (1979, p. 378) stated, "The safest conclusion at the present time is that human LTM can probably be represented as a node-link memory with an EPAM like index, but that various alternatives are still open for the detailed structure and organization of that memory." Subsequently, Lang- ley and Simon (198 1, p. 363) identified "recognition by means of discrimi- *Work on this paper was supported by a NSF Graduate Fellowship to the first author and by NIMH Grant 13950 to the second. We are grateful to Irving Biederman, Jeffrey Farrar, Janet Kolodner, Christopher Riesbeck. Brian Ross, Daniel Sewell, and Edward Smith for help- ful comments. The final product does not necessarily represent their positions on its Content.

Parts of this paper were presented at the Annual Stanford-Berkeley Experimental Psychology Conference, Stanford University, 1980. Requests for reprints should be addressed to Lawrence W. Barsalou, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322.

2 BARSALOU AND BOWER

nation nets" as one of several "possible invariants of human cognition."

Cognitive scientists have used EPAM-like discrimination nets for the last 25 years to model how humans access information in long term memory. After describing EPAM nets and some oftheir applications, we argue that they have a number of deficiencies as models of long term memory access. We next consider simple variants of EPAM discrimination nets and argue that they too lack psychological validity. We then show that simple parallel indexing mechanisms such as PANDEMONIUM avoid the problems of discrimination nets. Finally, we consider each model's use of test contingency in property acquisition and memory search, and we argue that fundamental differences in test contingency underly these models' differences in performance.

EPAM

EPAM (Elementary Perceiver and Memorizer) was developed by Feigenbaum and Simon to account for "elementary human symbolic learning processes" (e.g., Feigenbaum, 1963, p. 297; Simon & Feigenbaum, 1964). The central component of EPAM is an extremely efficient mechanism-a discrimination net-that sorts stimulus patterns to their correct actions on the basis of stimulus properties. Consider the example of an EPAM net in Figure 1. On the left is a list of stimulus-action pairs, in which letters represent properties of stimulus patterns and numbers represent actions. These could be the actual materials for a paired-associates task in which subjects learn to recall the number that goes with each letter string. We will use these pairs, however, to represent any kind of stimulus-action unit of behavior (e.g., object-category pairs in pattern recognition, cue-target pairs in memory retrieval, condition-action pairs in skilled performance).

The discrimination net on the right of Figure 1 generates the correct action for each stimulus. The non-terminal nodes in the net are tests of stimulus properties. If the stimulus EF is presented, it is first tested for having an E. Since the outcome is positive, EF is then tested for G. Failure on this test sorts EF to the terminal node that represents the correct action, 49. For AB, tests for E and then C fail, leading to the correct action, 62. We should note that this EPAM net cannot discriminate between stimuli that contain different permutations of the same properties (e.g., EG and GE). EPAM nets can easily make such discriminations, however, by making tests position specific. Instead of testing a stimulus for an E and a G in any position, a net could test it for an E in the first position and a G in the second position. An example of such a net is shown later at the bottom of Figure 5.

An attractive feature of discrimination nets is their ability to learn discriminations. Consider how the net in Figure 1 would learn the new pair, EH-50. When first presented with EH, a test succeeds at E but not at G, and

DISCRIMINATION NETS





Figure 1. An example of on EPAM discrimination net.

the net erroneously produces 49. To differentiate the two stimuli sorted to the same terminal node, the net grows a new test node based on a property of EH that does not yet exist on its sorting path in the net. In this case, H has not yet been used and is added to the bottom of the net where 49 once was. The old and new actions (49 and 50) are attached to the negative and positive branches. The net is now capable of responding correctly to both EF and EH.

A crucial component of EPAM is a "noticing order," prespecified by the program's designer, that can control (a) the order in which the properties of a stimulus are tested, and/or (b) the order in which the properties of a stimulus are added to its sorting path. One possibility for stimuli that are letter strings is a left-to-right order: When the properties of a stimulus are tested, they are tested from left to right; when a property is to be added to the sorting path of a stimulus, the left-most property not currently o n the path is chosen. As we shall see, there are many possible noticing orders.

EPAM nets have played a central role in computer programs whose primary goal is to simulate human cognitive processes. EPAM nets have successfully modelled a variety of verbal learning phenomena (Feigenbaum, 1963, 1965; Hintzman, 1968; Simon & Feigenbaum, 1964). Simon and Gilmartin (1973) used an EPAM net to recognize chess patterns in a simulation of chess perception, and Goldman used an EPAM net as part of a speech production simulation in Schank's (1975) conceptual dependency system.

Although the efficiency of EPAM nets makes them attractive to artificial intelligenceprojects, we have some reservations about their psychological validity. We now present the reasons that underly our lack of confidence.' IS. R. Anderson and G. H. Bower (1973) also provide a critique of EPAM,but it largely addresses shortcomings related to their specific interests.

BARSALOU AND BOWER

Negative Properties

The first problem with EPAM nets is their heavy reliance on negative properties as seen in the net in Figure 2. The stimulus for action 67 is defined in the net as not having an A, not having a B, and not having a D. It is defined solely by negative properties, that is, by properties it does not have. In any EPAM net, half the properties are negative, and only one stimulus can be recognized on the basis of all positive prop.erties..We find this counter-intuitive and psychologically implausible for seviral reasons.

–  –  –

Figure 2. An EPAM net that exhibits the problems of negative properties, insufficientsensitivity to the discriminativeness of properties, and extreme sensitivity to missing or incorrect properties.

First, people do not seem to have extensive knowledge of negative properties explicitly represented in memory (although they can compute them when necessary). When subjects list the properties of stimuli, they rarely, if ever, provide negative properties (see the norms of Ashcraft, 1978;

Hemenway, 1981; Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson, & Boyes-Braem, 1975).

Instead, people appear to primarily represent negative properties that deny normative expectations (e.g., the property of no vision for the concept of blindness).

Second, it seems counter-intuitive that people typically recognize things using negative information. This would be similar to walking into a room and identifying something as a chair because it does not fly and does

DISCRIMINATION NETS

not have gills. People primarily appear to recognize things using positive information. Notably, the major models of stimulus structure proposed by psychologists do not employ negative properties (e.g., the geometric model of Shepard, 1962a, 1962b; the feature model of Tversky, 1977). Moreover, models like these do an acceptable job of accounting for pattern recognition data (e.g., Appleman & Mayzner, 1982; Gibson, 1965; Keren & Baggen, 1981; Krumhansl, 1982; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1982).

A third problem with negative properties is that nets using them do not credit people with as much knowledge of stimulus patterns as they undoubtedly have. Consider the stimulus for action 67 in Figure 2. The net could tell us nothing positive about the properties of that object; yet it would be surprising if someone who had correctly learned its action could not recognize any of its positive properties. The use of negative properties is so computationally powerful that not all positive properties in a knowledge domain need to be represented. As the line labelled positive properties in Figure 2 shows, this net represents only 7 of the 19 positive properties in the domain. Yet we would be quite surprised if someone who had learned all the pairs in Figure 2 could recognize less than half the properties. We suspect most of the properties would have become established in memory and could be recognized as having occurred within the domain.

Attempts have been made to some extent to remedy this lack-of-knowledge problem. The nets in Feigenbaum (1965) initially sorted stimuli to stimulus images instead of to actions. These images were presumed to build up with repeated experience until they eventually contained all a pattern's positive properties. To produce an action, a stimulus image was subsequently presented to the net and sorted to its action. But this appears to be quite redundant. Why should pattern recognition include both the classification of a percept and the subsequent classification of its memory representation, which is in some sense a copy? Representing a stimulus three times-once in the net that classifies the percept, once as a stimulus image, and once in the net that classifies the stimulus image-seems unnatural and inefficient. It would be more reasonable to use a single representation for identifying a pattern and for storing knowledge about its properties.

Hintman (1968) proposed a second solution to the lack-of-knowledge problem. In one version of his SAL model, overlearning was permitted such that all a pattern's positive properties could eventually be encoded as tests in its net. But this addition violates the spirit of EPAM nets. It undercuts their claim to fast, efficient knowledge indexing based on very little information.

Clearly, if all the positive properties of a domain are eventually represented, there is no reason in the first place to represent negative properties. It would seem preferable to use some other, more efficient representation that contains only positive properties. Notably, human subjects, on occasion, do

6 BARSALOU AND BOWER

learn more information than is necessary to discriminate stimuli (Trabasso & Bower, 1968). Consequently, some form of overlearning is necessary to account for the acquisition of redundant discriminative properties.

A fourth problem with negative properties is that nets with suchproperties fail to capture the distinction between generating an action to a previously perceived stimulus and generating the same action to a completely novel stimulus. The net in Figure 2, for example, would produce the same action to unfamiliar stimuli such as WXY, GJK, and FEG as to the familiar stimulus QRS, and would have no way of determining which is the familiar pattern. Yet people would probably be much better at recognizing familiar patterns than unfamiliar ones that lead to the same action.

A fifth problem with negative properties is that nets with these properties form unintuitive equivalence classes. The patterns that could be sorted to 67 in Figure 2, for example, surely form a peculiar category. This category could include familiar patterns (e.g., QRS), new patterns whose properties have occurred in the domain before (e.g., FEG), and new patterns whose properties have never occurred in the domain (e..g., WXY). Quite often these categories violate the fundamental classification principle of maximizing withincategory similarity and minimizing between-category similarity. As an illustration, consider how the net in Figure 2 would categorize the new stimulus GJK. According to the classification principle just stated, GJK should be classified with AJK, but the net instead classifies it with QRS. Thus, the use of negative properties leads to predictions about transfer performance that can be unintuitive and inconsistent with psychological data.

Insufficient Sensitivity to the Discriminativeness of Properties



Pages:   || 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |


Similar works:

«CANCER SUPPORT SERVICES in Ireland DIRECTORY Leinster Irish Cancer Society DIRECTORY OF SERVICES Welcome Welcome to the Fourth Edition of the Directory of Cancer Support Services Affiliated to the Irish Cancer Society. More people are getting cancer in Ireland. It is estimated that one in three of us will be diagnosed with the disease during our lifetime. Cancer is however increasingly viewed as a condition from which people survive. Evidence shows that when people experiencing cancer receive...»

«General-purpose localization of textured • • Image regions Rutb Rosenboltz· XeroxPARC 3333 Coyote Hill Rd. Palo Alto, CA 94304 Abstract We suggest a working definition of texture: Texture is stuff that is more compactly represented by its statistics than by specifying the configuration of its parts. This definition suggests that to fmd texture we look for outliers to the local statistics, and label as texture the regions with no outliers. We present a method, based upon this idea, for...»

«TECHNISCHE UNIVERSITÄT MÜNCHEN Lehrstuhl für Aquatische Systembiologie Bioindication tools for measuring the success of stream restoration Joachim Wolfgang Pander Vollständiger Abdruck der von der Fakultät Wissenschaftszentrum Weihenstephan für Ernährung, Landnutzung und Umwelt der Technischen Universität München zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften genehmigten Dissertation. Vorsitzender: Univ.-Prof. Dr. A. Melzer Prüfer der Dissertation: 1....»

«Tiedemann, K. B., D. F. Ventura and C. Ades. 1986. Spectral sensitivities of the eyes of the orb we b spider Argiope argentata (Fabricius). J. Arachnol., 14 :71-78. SPECTRAL SENSITIVITIES OF THE EYES OF THE ORB WE B SPIDER ARGIOPE ARGENTATA (FABRICIUS ) Klaus B. Tiedemann, Dora Fix Ventura and Cesar Ade s Department of Experimental Psychology Institute of Psychology University of Sao Paul o Sao Paulo, Brazil ABSTRACT Spectral sensitivity curves to light between 425 and 650 nm for the...»

«1 Sociocultural Issues in Learning English for Women in Northwest Pakistan Dissertation zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors Englische Philologie am Fachbereich Philosophie und Geisteswissenschaften der Freien Universität Berlin vorgelegt von Sabina Shah Berlin, February, 2015 2 Erstgutachter/in: Prof. Dr. Gerhard Leitner (Institut für Englische Philologie) Zweitgutachter/in: Prof. Dr. Ferdinand von Mengden (Institut für Englische Philologie) Tag der Disputation: 14. Juli 2015 3 Contents...»

«What Indoor Cats Need To enrich the lives of indoor cats, we have developed this resource checklist; and some suggestions for making changes. Informed Owners As an owner, one of the most important things you can do for you cat is to educate yourself about feline idiosyncrasies. These resources will help you do just that. Books From the Cat's Point of View answers nearly every question the new cat owner could have and gives the experienced cat owner a look at life from the other side of the...»

«Begin Reading Table of Contents Copyright Page In accordance with the U.S. Copyright Act of 1976, the scanning, uploading, and electronic sharing of any part of this book without the permission of the publisher constitute unlawful piracy and theft of the author’s intellectual property. If you would like to use material from the book (other than for review purposes), prior written permission must be obtained by contacting the publisher at permissions@hbgusa.com. Thank you for your support of...»

«Fools Die Mario Puzo Book I Chapter 1 “Listen to me. I will tell you the truth about a man’s life. I will tell you the truth about his love for women. That he never hates them. Already you think I’m on the wrong track. Stay with me. Really—I’m a master of magic. “Do you believe a man can truly love a woman and constantly betray her? Never mind physically, but betray her in his mind, in the very ‘poetry of his soul.’ Well, it’s not easy, but men do it all the time. “Do you...»

«Louisiana Architecture: 1945-1965 The Contemporary House NATIONAL BACKGROUND Introduction: “I am not going to sit on furniture that continually reminds me of a machine shop or a hospital operating room.” Albert Einstein Machined modernism in art and architecture wasn’t for everyone. If Einstein had been talking about houses, he might have had in mind the high-art Farnsworth House near Chicago, a serene steel and glass box designed by the supreme modernist Mies van der Rohe (Photo 1). But...»

«29 Hacker Perspectives on Privacy “It Doesn’t Have to Be This Way”: Hacker Perspectives on Privacy Kevin Steinmetz & Jurg Gerber* We face a lot of troubling times ahead with regards to surveillance. Most of the power, for the moment at least, remains in our hands and in our minds, should we choose to use them. It is our acceptance of the elements of a surveillance state which will give it the most strength and solidify its presence for future generations. It doesn’t have to be this way....»

«*** Free EBOOK*** Your Journey of Personal Transformation Begins HERE: An Introduction to Understanding Your Human Design Chart by Evelyn Levenson, MBA Certified Human Design Specialist Your Success By Design Coach Your Journey Begins HERE www.HumanDesignforSuccess.com This Report is for your personal use. You MAY SHARE it, in its entirety with all author and contact information. However, You May NOT Trade, Sell or Resell This Intellectual Property in Any Way. You may not reprint any part of it...»

«From Soulard’s Notebooks Assistant Editor: Kassandra Soulard Poetry by Ric Amante 1 Notes from New England [Commentary] by Raymond Soulard, Jr. 9 Poetry by Judih Haggai 27 Poetry by Joe Ciccone 32 Paelo Redemption [Travel Journal] by Charlie Beyer 35 Poetry by Joe Coleman 45 Baby Steps Up into the Sky [Travel Journal] by Nathan D. Horowitz 55 Many Musics [Poetry] by Raymond Soulard, Jr. 59 Psychedelics and Lucid Dreaming: Doorways in the Mind [Essay] by A.S. Kay 91 Poetry by Tom Sheehan 99...»





 
<<  HOME   |    CONTACTS
2016 www.dissertation.xlibx.info - Dissertations, online materials

Materials of this site are available for review, all rights belong to their respective owners.
If you do not agree with the fact that your material is placed on this site, please, email us, we will within 1-2 business days delete him.